Ting the poor readers. Talcott et al. speculated that greater motion energy might be expected to facilitate performance if motion sensors have a relatively low response gain, more inherent noise or sparser spatial sampling but no firm conclusions could be drawn. An alternative hypothesis is that deficits on sensory tasks associated with poor reading and dyslexia are the result of impairments in external-noise exclusion (Sperling, Lu, Manis, Seidenberg, 2005). Within this framework relatively poor perfor-mance on RDK tasks, in which coherence thresholds are used as a measure of sensitivity, is directly indicative of an underlying problem in segregating the signal dots from the noise dots. Although this noise-exclusion hypothesis has received support (e.g. Sperling, Lu, Manis, Seidenberg, 2006) it fails to explain why some individuals with dyslexia often exhibit relatively normal performance on analogous static global form tasks that also contain high levels of visual noise. For example, Hansen, Stein, Orde, Winter, and Talcott (2001) administered two psychophysical tasks: a buy AZD-8055 random-dot global motion task and a static global form task. The latter was devised by Atkinson et al. (1997) to investigate the processing of global form in individuals with Williams syndrome. It is assumed to provide a sensitive measure of ventral stream capability because it evokes a BOLD response in cortical areas that have been implicated in the processing of global form (Braddick, O’Brien, Wattam-Bell, Atkinson, Turner, 2000). The stimuli in the task are similar to the random-dot patterns described above, except they comprise static line segments rather than dots. They can either be orientated coherently to form a concentric target or randomly. Poor readers’ coherence thresholds were significantly higher than those of controls on the random-dot global motion task but not the static global form task. This result is difficult to reconcile with a general noise-exclusion hypothesis but is consistent with the dorsal stream vulnerability hypothesis. A related issue concerns the degree to which motion segmentation processes are normal fpsyg.2017.00209 in individuals with dyslexia. This is important because under natural viewing the visual system has to satisfy the competing requirements of integrating local motion signals that belong to a common surface or object but also segregating those arising from other objects in the world (e.g. Braddick, 1993). How the visual system achieves this delicate balance is still unknown but there is some evidence to suggest that motion segmentation mechanisms may also be impaired in poor readers. Hill and Raymond (2002) investigated this issue using transparent motion stimuli generated by constraining half of the dots in a RDK to move coherently in a horizontal direction (leftwards or rightwards) and others to move vertically (upwards or downwards). This created the perception of two segregated and transparent surfaces sliding across each other and the subjects’ task was to identify the two directions of motion present on each trial. The TAK-385 site exposure duration of the stimulus was manipulated by changing the number of images comprising the motion sequence. A transparency threshold was calculated, which corresponded to the minimum exposure duration needed to achieve 75 correct SART.S23503 performance. Results showed that poor readers’ transparency thresholds were over three times higher than those of controls in that they required an additional 339 ms to identify.Ting the poor readers. Talcott et al. speculated that greater motion energy might be expected to facilitate performance if motion sensors have a relatively low response gain, more inherent noise or sparser spatial sampling but no firm conclusions could be drawn. An alternative hypothesis is that deficits on sensory tasks associated with poor reading and dyslexia are the result of impairments in external-noise exclusion (Sperling, Lu, Manis, Seidenberg, 2005). Within this framework relatively poor perfor-mance on RDK tasks, in which coherence thresholds are used as a measure of sensitivity, is directly indicative of an underlying problem in segregating the signal dots from the noise dots. Although this noise-exclusion hypothesis has received support (e.g. Sperling, Lu, Manis, Seidenberg, 2006) it fails to explain why some individuals with dyslexia often exhibit relatively normal performance on analogous static global form tasks that also contain high levels of visual noise. For example, Hansen, Stein, Orde, Winter, and Talcott (2001) administered two psychophysical tasks: a random-dot global motion task and a static global form task. The latter was devised by Atkinson et al. (1997) to investigate the processing of global form in individuals with Williams syndrome. It is assumed to provide a sensitive measure of ventral stream capability because it evokes a BOLD response in cortical areas that have been implicated in the processing of global form (Braddick, O’Brien, Wattam-Bell, Atkinson, Turner, 2000). The stimuli in the task are similar to the random-dot patterns described above, except they comprise static line segments rather than dots. They can either be orientated coherently to form a concentric target or randomly. Poor readers’ coherence thresholds were significantly higher than those of controls on the random-dot global motion task but not the static global form task. This result is difficult to reconcile with a general noise-exclusion hypothesis but is consistent with the dorsal stream vulnerability hypothesis. A related issue concerns the degree to which motion segmentation processes are normal fpsyg.2017.00209 in individuals with dyslexia. This is important because under natural viewing the visual system has to satisfy the competing requirements of integrating local motion signals that belong to a common surface or object but also segregating those arising from other objects in the world (e.g. Braddick, 1993). How the visual system achieves this delicate balance is still unknown but there is some evidence to suggest that motion segmentation mechanisms may also be impaired in poor readers. Hill and Raymond (2002) investigated this issue using transparent motion stimuli generated by constraining half of the dots in a RDK to move coherently in a horizontal direction (leftwards or rightwards) and others to move vertically (upwards or downwards). This created the perception of two segregated and transparent surfaces sliding across each other and the subjects’ task was to identify the two directions of motion present on each trial. The exposure duration of the stimulus was manipulated by changing the number of images comprising the motion sequence. A transparency threshold was calculated, which corresponded to the minimum exposure duration needed to achieve 75 correct SART.S23503 performance. Results showed that poor readers’ transparency thresholds were over three times higher than those of controls in that they required an additional 339 ms to identify.